What is transsexualism ?
The first level of expression of
transsexualism is cross-dressing (transvestitism). The second is hormone therapy. The third is the sex-change operation. Some authors call this condition "gender
dysphoria". Basically a
transsexual man feels like a woman and wishes he could look like one and live
like one. The inverse, female-to-male
transsexualism, also occurs, less
frequently. I feel a bit uncomfortable
with the term "gender dysphoria",
and yet recognize its utility as well.
The term literally means dissatisfaction with the sexual appearance of
one's body. So in fact, the term
highlights the plight of transsexuals,
since the only full liberation available to them is major surgery and
lifelong hormone treatment -not to
mention the necessity of rebuilding one's interpersonal life. On the other hand, some transsexuals have expressed extreme
satisfaction after having "changed" their sex, so that I find the term "gender
dysphoria" a bit superficial since it does not necessarily apply. Research on transsexuals (Doorn, Poortinga et
Verschoor, 1994) has found that the condition manifests
itself far earlier than does sexual orientation (homosexuality or
heterosexuality). Prevalence of
transsexualism is estimated, based conservatively on surgical sex changes, at
one case in 22,100 births (Bakker,
Van-Kesteren, Gooren et Bezemer, 1993).
The same study determined that males are more often transsexual than
females by a factor of 2.5. Most transsexuals identify
themselves as such, and can usually be
so identified by others, during the
preschool years. The relation between
sexual identity and sexual orientation is not as simple as one would
think. One would expect that if a man
feels like a woman inside, then he will
be attracted to men in a manner similar to the way women are -especially if he undergoes the sex-change operation. We would expect the inverse for transsexual
women of course. However, one study (Devor, 1993) found that only about forty percent of operated transsexuals had
more sexual relations with people of the opposite genotype and body
phenotype! However, as if this weren't
complicated enough, a sizable proportion
of the sexual partners of transsexuals are homosexually oriented (see Dickey et Stephens,
1995). Finally,
it is not unusual for transsexuals to be sexual partners autres (Blanchard et
Collins, 1993). What I make of this is that with the uprooted
life and ambiguous anatomy of transsexuals,
they often end up with sexual partners who have high tolerance for
sexual ambiguity or atypicality and cultural disparity. However it has clearly been documented that
some transsexuals actually have a sexual identity of the opposite sex (by
definition) and a homosexual orientation within this sexual identity. One study documented, very explicitly, two
cases of genotypic men, who felt and lived like women, and were sexually attracted, very
preferentially, to women. The proportion
of such cases is increasing with each survey,
such that the latest most credible surveys indicate that 50% of
genotypically male tanssexuals are sexually attracted primarily to women. This proves that there is a substantial
difference between, and dissociability of,
sexual orientation and sexual identity. According to Ray Blanchard, evidence indicates that bisexual, asexual,
and heterosexual male gender dysphorics are similar to each other (the
autogynephilia type), and dissimilar to homosexual male gender dysphorics (the
androphilia type), with regard to a history of transvestic fetishism, degree of
recalled childhood femininity, age at clinical presentation, extent of
interpersonal heterosexual experience, and a history of erotic arousal in association
with the thought of being a woman (for details, see Blanchard, 1985, 1988,
1989a, 1989b).
I am saddened to have to say that very little is known about the biological basis of
transsexualism. Surely the major
difficulty preventing rapid advance of our neurobiological knowledge of
transsexualism is the fact that it would be extremely difficult, if not silly or just crude, to try to develop an animal model of sexual
identity. There are of course a large
array of sex-specific behaviors, often
very different from one species to the other,
but these are usually related to reproduction (territorial, parental,
sexual behaviors). At any rate, the most interesting aspect of human
transsexualism, the subjective gender
dysphoria, cannot be estimated, nor does it probably exist, in lower animals.
The first level of expression of
transsexualism is cross-dressing (transvestitism). The second is hormone therapy. The third is the sex-change operation. Some authors call this condition "gender
dysphoria". Basically a
transsexual man feels like a woman and wishes he could look like one and live
like one. The inverse, female-to-male
transsexualism, also occurs, less
frequently. I feel a bit uncomfortable
with the term "gender dysphoria",
and yet recognize its utility as well.
The term literally means dissatisfaction with the sexual appearance of
one's body. So in fact, the term
highlights the plight of transsexuals,
since the only full liberation available to them is major surgery and
lifelong hormone treatment -not to
mention the necessity of rebuilding one's interpersonal life. On the other hand, some transsexuals have expressed extreme
satisfaction after having "changed" their sex, so that I find the term "gender
dysphoria" a bit superficial since it does not necessarily apply. Research on transsexuals (Doorn, Poortinga et
Verschoor, 1994) has found that the condition manifests
itself far earlier than does sexual orientation (homosexuality or
heterosexuality). Prevalence of
transsexualism is estimated, based conservatively on surgical sex changes, at
one case in 22,100 births (Bakker,
Van-Kesteren, Gooren et Bezemer, 1993).
The same study determined that males are more often transsexual than
females by a factor of 2.5. Most transsexuals identify
themselves as such, and can usually be
so identified by others, during the
preschool years. The relation between
sexual identity and sexual orientation is not as simple as one would
think. One would expect that if a man
feels like a woman inside, then he will
be attracted to men in a manner similar to the way women are -especially if he undergoes the sex-change operation. We would expect the inverse for transsexual
women of course. However, one study (Devor, 1993) found that only about forty percent of operated transsexuals had
more sexual relations with people of the opposite genotype and body
phenotype! However, as if this weren't
complicated enough, a sizable proportion
of the sexual partners of transsexuals are homosexually oriented (see Dickey et Stephens,
1995). Finally,
it is not unusual for transsexuals to be sexual partners autres (Blanchard et
Collins, 1993). What I make of this is that with the uprooted
life and ambiguous anatomy of transsexuals,
they often end up with sexual partners who have high tolerance for
sexual ambiguity or atypicality and cultural disparity. However it has clearly been documented that
some transsexuals actually have a sexual identity of the opposite sex (by
definition) and a homosexual orientation within this sexual identity. One study documented, very explicitly, two
cases of genotypic men, who felt and lived like women, and were sexually attracted, very
preferentially, to women. The proportion
of such cases is increasing with each survey,
such that the latest most credible surveys indicate that 50% of
genotypically male tanssexuals are sexually attracted primarily to women. This proves that there is a substantial
difference between, and dissociability of,
sexual orientation and sexual identity. According to Ray Blanchard, evidence indicates that bisexual, asexual,
and heterosexual male gender dysphorics are similar to each other (the
autogynephilia type), and dissimilar to homosexual male gender dysphorics (the
androphilia type), with regard to a history of transvestic fetishism, degree of
recalled childhood femininity, age at clinical presentation, extent of
interpersonal heterosexual experience, and a history of erotic arousal in association
with the thought of being a woman (for details, see Blanchard, 1985, 1988,
1989a, 1989b).
I am saddened to have to say that very little is known about the biological basis of
transsexualism. Surely the major
difficulty preventing rapid advance of our neurobiological knowledge of
transsexualism is the fact that it would be extremely difficult, if not silly or just crude, to try to develop an animal model of sexual
identity. There are of course a large
array of sex-specific behaviors, often
very different from one species to the other,
but these are usually related to reproduction (territorial, parental,
sexual behaviors). At any rate, the most interesting aspect of human
transsexualism, the subjective gender
dysphoria, cannot be estimated, nor does it probably exist, in lower animals.
Post Comment
No comments