Is the left hemisphere more developed in human females than males ?
There is no argument in the scientific
literature against the view that girls and women are superior on some verbal
skills and that boys and men are better on certain visuospatial skills. The first logical explanation of this that
ought to come to mind would be that females have a larger, more developed left
hemisphere and males have a larger, more developed right hemisphere. After all,
nothing is more solidly established in neuropsychology than the fact
that verbal functions are left hemisphere-specialized and visuospatial
functions are right hemisphere-specialized.
In a certain sense, one famous
author actually made that argument and managed to convince a lot of people that
his argument was valid. Norman
Geschwind, and a few of his colleagues
proposed that prenatal testosterone actually slows down the development of the
left hemisphere -but only at a certain
period of its development. This
roundabout theory allowed him to eschew evidence against a basic sex-specific
anatomical difference between whole hemispheres. Is there any evidence of whole-hemisphere
sex differences at all then ? One very
suggestive and exciting sex difference has been observed, as far as I know,
only in rats. Male and female rats have
opposite asymmetries in cortical thickness on the left and right sides of their
brains. However, the female rat, if ovariectomized before birth, manifests the
male-typical asymmetry as an adult !
An anatomist named Crichton-Brown had observed in 1880
that the two brain hemispheres of women are of similar weight, whereas,
in men the right hemisphere is heavier.
One research team reported that the right hemisphere of the human male
fetus is larger than the left, and that
this is not the case of the female human fetus.
Do these anatomical brain differences have a significant impact on
cognitive abilities and behavior of normal men and women ? It is still too early to say. However,
as usual, answers to these types
of questions are obtained more revealingly from pathology than from research on
normal subjects. There are two data
bases which provide information about this issue, IQ subscale profiles and aphasia, following unilateral hemispheric lesions.
A very large meta-analysis was published in 1982
by researchers named Inglis and
Lawson. They found that right hemisphere
lesions affected performance (non-verbal) IQ more than verbal IQ, in both sexes. However,
left hemisphere lesions affected verbal IQ more than performance IQ only
in men. There have been several other studies of the effects of unilateral
brain lesions on IQs of men and women.
It has consistently been found that the results suggest more
lateralisation of function in men than in women. Verbal IQ is more selectively affected by
left hemisphere lesions in men than in women,
and Performance (less verbal,
more visuospatial) IQ is more
selectively affected by right hemisphere lesions in men than in women.
Women who suffer cerebrovascular accidents
(obstructions or ruptures of arteries feeding the brain) in the left hemisphere
become less frequently aphasic (unable to understand or produce speech) than do men.
Two explanations of the aphasia and IQ effects of hemispheric lesions as
a function of gender are currently being debated. Language functions could be represented in a
more distributed fashion within the left hemisphere in women, or language functions could be distributed to
a greater extent in the two hemispheres in women, whereas in men it could be more constrained
to the left hemisphere. One argument
for the former point of view is research on the effect of barbiturization of a
whole hemisphere. An anesthetic called
sodium amytal is sometimes injected into a catheter located in one of the
carotid arteries, the main artery
irrigating the brain hemisphere, in
epileptic patients. This procedure
serves to determine to what extent the non-anesthetized hemisphere can support
language and/or memory functions. It is
used for epileptics who do not respond well to anticonvulsant drugs. If it turns out that language or most memory
functions are localized on the side of the brain that the surgeon wanted to
resect (remove), the operation is
canceled. If the patient is lucky
enough to have enough language and memory functions in the hemisphere not
requiring resection, he or she is likely
to be relieved of his or her epileptic seizures after the resection of the part
of the brain containing the epileptic focus.
This diagnostic technique is also known as the Wada technique, after its
inventor. Now the Wada procedure is
extremely useful for the study of hemispheric specialization. Large cohorts of men and women epileptics
have now been tested for language functions after anesthesia of the entire left
or right hemisphere. It turns out that
men and women have very similar profiles:
they make very few verbal errors and understand well what is
communicated to them when it is their right hemisphere that is
anesthetized, but they make a lot of
mistakes and understand communication poorly when it is their left hemisphere
that is anesthetized.
Finally, a
third position on the issue of sex differences in hemispheric asymmetry has
been formulated by Kimura. She reports
that women manifest aphasia more frequently than men following left frontal
lobe lesions, whereas men do so more
frequently following lesions in the posterior part of the left hemisphere. Kimura believes the left hemisphere is
specialized for several important aspects of motor programming, thus the high prevalence of right handedness
and left hemisphere based speech as well,
in both sexes. She found that, as
was the case for aphasia, manual apraxia
(loss of the understanding or expression of learned hand gestures) results more frequently in women from left
frontal lesions, and in men from left
posterior lesions. She also makes the
point that in those aspects of speech in which women excel over men, it is in speed and precision of speech
production and fluency. Kimura thus
speculates that women’s cognitive strengths may boil down to a sort of
evolutionary specialization for fine motor coordination implanted in her brain
by natural selection of a home based ecology (child care, tool making, etc.)
My own position with regard to the literature on this
particular topic is that the whole hemisphere is not necessarily the proper
level of interpretation of human sex differences in the brain, except when the data being investigated
clearly pertain to the whole hemisphere.
Hemispheric barbiturization (anesthesia of a whole side of the
brain), hemispherectomy (removal of the
entire cortex of one hemisphere), electroencephalographic (EEG) topography (using electrodes placed
over the entire hemisphere), and
experiments involving electrical stimulation of a wide diversity of human
cortical surface areas (a diagnostic procedure in epilepsy), are examples of
techniques yielding data appropriate for inferences about interhemispheric sex
differences in terms of whole hemispheres.
Localized brain lesions,
tachistoscopic and dichotic listening experiments (see chapter 3), and anatomical analysis of individual brain
systems such as nuclei (closely knit neuronal networks) or fasciculi (large
pathways containing many neuronal axons) are techniques which may reveal sex
differences either between or within the hemispheres, thus requiring more prudence in neuropsychological
interpretation.
Post Comment
No comments