Sex differences in play behavior
It could be debated whether the neural
determinants of sex differences in play behavior are different from those
involved in the expression of aggressiveness and fear. At any rate,
there is little debate about the greater propensity of male mammals
(including humans) for « rough and tumble » play (Graves, 1978; Pellegrini,
1989; Rosenthal, 1983). In humans, this
male trait appears to be manifest in a widely differing cultures (Freedman et
al, 1979). The
human male infant even appears to be slightly precocious than the female in his
motor development, probably because of
this gender difference (Deldime
& Vermeulen, 1997; Reinisch et al,
1991). Sex
differences in social play in lower mammals, like rats, appear to be more
quantitative than qualitative, referring to frequency and not the form of the
behaviors -but in higher mammals there
are some clear qualitative differences as well.
For example, juvenile male primate social rank correlates with number of
peer social interactions, which predominantly take the form of play-fighting.
Females on the other hand appear to spend less time play-fighting and spend
more time waiting and competing for interactions with infants, i.e.
play-mothering, whereby they acquire the motor skills necessary for handling
infants (Erhardt,
1984).
Whereas increased perinatal exposure to exogenous
testosterone masculinizes social play, experimental manipulations of androgen
levels after this period (i.e. following critical periods for neuronal
differentiation) apparently have no effect on the expression of social play.
This effect appears to involve, at least in part, androgen receptor occupancy
in the amygdala. In the rat, there is a prominent sex difference in
nuclear-bound androgen receptors in the amygdala during the sensitive period
for the masculinization of play-fighting. Moreover, testosterone implants
directly into the amygdala during this period masculinize social play in
females (Meany
& McEwen, 1986). Though the amygdala has not yet
been investigated in this manner in monkeys as far as I could discern, prenatal androgenization of female macaques
has been reported to increase juvenile rough and tumble play (Goy et al, 1988). Perinatal androgen exposure may also be
important in humans, since girls born with congenital adrenal hyperplasia
diagnosed and treated at birth still show male-like patterns of play (Hines & Kaufman,
1994).
In one investigation the effects of altering neonatal
levels of progestins on the later development of social play behaviour was
studied in rats. Progestin levels were raised in experiment one by administering
injections of either progesterone or medroxyprogesterone acetate. Exposure to
either hormone led to reduced levels of social play in juvenile rats of both
sexes, confirming earlier reports of lowered levels of play following
medroxyprogesterone obtained via maternal milk. In a second experiment,
endogenous progestin levels were lowered by administration of the antiserum to
progesterone. The prediction that this should result in raised levels of
juvenile play was supported for males, but not for females. Females in by contrast showed a decrease in
play. The reduction of play behavior by
progestin in male rats has been reported by other investigators as well.
Theories concerning the function of sex differences in
social play often emphasize either the social or motor learning functions.
However, such differences may reflect socio-biological and developmental
cascades that are, in some way, initiated by perinatal hormonal events and then
further modulated by hormones throughout development.
Post Comment
No comments