What is the significance of the male preponderance of left handedness ?
It has long been known that there are more left
handed boys and men than there are left handed girls and women. I have found close to a dozen studies having
carefully investigated this issue, so
that we can now estimate the gender-specific prevalence rates over tens of
thousands of cases. The male/female
ratio of left handedness in these studies varied from 1.3:1 to 2:1 (the average
was 1.5:1) with every single study reporting higher prevalence in males, and the majority obtaining a statistically
significant effect. The higher
prevalence of right handedness in the female human is observed as early as it
can be measured. Prior to this, in infancy, motoric lateralization has to be
studied either by observing spontaneous movements, or by testing reflexes on each side of the
body. One investigation found that there
was a sex difference in asymmetry of three foot reflexes in newborns: females
were right biased, but the majority of males were left biased.
Handedness is highly hereditary. This influence is the primary one. Indeed heritability estimates derived from
concordance rates of homozygotic (from one ovum) and dizygotic (from two ova)
twins are very high. Also, it appears that the hereditary determination
of handedness can supercede the hormonal determination: there have been reports of cases of left and
right handers with androgen insensitivity syndrome. In this syndrome, every cell of the body is insensitive to
androgens, so that genetic males
actually look very much like females.
If testosterone were an important determinant of left handedness, as has been argued by Geschwind and his
colleagues, then all these cases should
be right handed.
A subtle prenatal hormonal modulation of handedness
certainly exists. Prenatally masculinized women, whether by synthetic estrogen (diethylstilbestrol, i.e., DES) or by adrenal hyperplasia are more
prone to left handedness (see chapter 7 for additional details). Likewise,
Nass and colleagues (1987) found that women masculinized by congenital
hyperplasia were more often left handed than their normal sisters.
However,
another factor may be involved.
To understand the neuropsychology of handedness, it is imperative to grasp that motor control
is contralateral in mammals. The left
hemisphere controls the muscles of the right side of the body, and the right hemisphere controls those on
the left. The male sex is more at risk
for prenatal diffuse brain damage (cerebral palsy, epilepsy, mental deficiency,
hydrocephalus) and focal brain damage as well (cerebrovascular accident,
localized dysplasia) (see chapter 6).
Diffuse brain damage is associated with left handedness, independently of gender. For example, there are also slightly more
left handers among stutterers,
dyslexics, mental
retardates, and autists. Incidentally,
these are all male-prevalent conditions. It is supposed that early disruptions of
brain development place the individual at risk for deviation of a delicate
bifurcation leading to right or left handedness. Focal lesions have a more dramatic
effect. When the prenatal or perinatal
lesion is in the right hemisphere, the
bearer of the lesion is even more likely to become a right hander than
otherwise. However, when the lesion is "strategically
emplaced" in the left hemisphere,
motor control functions may shift to the opposite hemisphere, leading to left handedness. In either case, the male sex is more at risk.
In short, there
are in fact three causes of left handedness:
heredity, prenatal testosterone,
and brain damage -and of
course, these three factors interact.
There is no statistically significant difference
between intelligence on IQ tests between left handers and right handers. The next logical question would be whether
left and right handers differ as to cognitive profiles (verbal versus
visuospatial). Unfortunately, IQ tests are not very adequate for answering
this question: IQ test items were
constructed or selected, at outset, so as to reduce, as much as possible, sex differences, and this had the effect of also reducing differences
between left and right handers. It is
more relevant, I think, to look at research which selected tests designed to
measure hemispherically specialized functions -which is not the case of IQ
tests. One set of studies carried out
by Harshman and colleagues found that left handed men perform worse that right
handed men on visuospatial (right hemisphere) tasks and yet better on certain
verbal (left hemisphere tasks).
However, these results could
only be extracted when “reasoning ability” had been partialed out (i.e.,
statistically controlled). While these
studies are important and useful, it is
most pertinent, I think, to review
investigations exploiting the two experimental neuropsychology techniques I
outline in chapter 3: dichotic listening
and tachistoscopy. A neuropsychologist
called Jerry Levy has posited that women’s brain organization in charge of
cognition resembles that of male left handers.
Indeed, dichotic listening
asymmetries and half-field effects of left handers generally resemble those of
women. Levy proposed that cognitive
functions are more bilaterally represented in women and that they are more
hemispherically lateralized in men: verbal functions being located more in the
left hemisphere and visuospatial functions being located more in the right
hemisphere. She also argued that the
visuospatial function is generally more efficient when it eschews verbal
strategies. In other words, she stated that the visuospatial function is
better processed by a specialized network located in one hemisphere and
shielded from verbal interference (men),
than by a non-specialized network located in both hemispheres
(women). In short, the neuropsychological significance of left
handedness is very perplexing: though men are more often left handed, left handers have cognitive profiles like
women.
Sandra Witelson has proposed an intriguing
neurodevelopmental model which seeks to explain sex differences not only in
handedness but also in hemispheric asymmetry. She developed the hypothesis that
naturally occurring loss of axons of the corpus callosum (either symmetric or
asymmetric, with or without neuron death) may be one mechanism underlying the
embryological development of hand preference and hemispheric anatomical and
functional asymmetries in males. Specifically,
she suggests that there is a subtle deficit in natural neuronal
mortality, in healthy pruning of the
male brain, during gestation. Supporting evidence for this hypothesis is
noted from a report of increased prevalence of left-handedness in children born
prematurely at the gestational age prior to the likely onset of axon loss.
Animal research into motoric asymmetry is globally
concordant with the human handedness literature. Indeed female rats are generally more motorically
lateralized than the males. Though
there is no overall right or left paw preference in rats, each animal can be shown to prefer one or the
other paw -when required to reach into
a container for food. Females show
stronger preference for their preferred paw.
In other words, they are more
consistently lateralized. Postural
asymmetry is easy to measure in rats:
the tail is either to the right or to the left. Here also,
females are more lateralized.
Rotational behavior can also be measured in terms of motoric
asymmetry: rats turn more to the left or
to the right. Females are more
consistently asymmetric. These sex differences
(which we suppose operate in the brain) are mediated by gonadal hormones. Indeed,
gonadectomy attenuates the asymmetry in females but has no effect in
males. Estradiol injections and
naturally occurring estrous both increase the turning asymmetry observed in
females.
Post Comment
No comments