Sex differences having to do with sexual behavior
One
trait which distinguishes the male from the female mammal, in most species, is the greater libido (sex drive) of males. Some people would, I think, disagree with that statement. Some of the reasons why I think men have a
greater libido, at least as far as its
biological dimension is concerned, are the following: 1) large scale inventories have shown that
desire for more sex on the part of the husband from the wife is higher, on the
average, than on the part of the wife
from the husband (by about 15%), 2) the same inventories have revealed that
boys and men masturbate more than girls and women do, 3) boys (and late treated
androgenital girls, i.e., prenatally hormonally masculinized girls, see the next sections of this chapter for
more details) have erotic dreams in adolescence prior to any concrete sexual
experience, whereas this happens to
women only in their twenties, and 4) temporal lobe epileptics who report
epileptogenic erotic or orgastic «hallucinations» are far more frequently, and
disproportionately, of the male sex.
One interesting aspect of the libido of men and women is that its
life-course trajectory differs slightly.
Of course, the sex drive declines
in both sexes throughout adulthood, a
phenomenon apparently related (among other things) to the general decline in
steroid hormone concentrations in both sexes.
However, it seems that the
decline is a bit steeper for men from around age 30 to around age 45. Then menopause inflects the female trajectory
toward lower libido. Even the erotic
content of dreams in women could be modulated by hormones: women have been found to have more erotic
dreams during pregnancy, a time when
their estrogen level is climbing up. Of
course, the duration of a sexual
relationship ends up being a sexual inhibitor, in most polygamous species, for both sexes.
By the way, it is an undeniable fact that the human
species, like most primates, is
polygamous -with a strong tendency for pair bonding -and I simply state this as a matter of
fact, not as a matter of morals. Most of humanity’s cultures, I think, have
been formally polygamous (for men).
Otherwise, the occurrence of
multiple partners in contemporary western society, as estimated from survey
research, is very high. The divorce and re-marriage rate is also very
high, and this particular statistic only
scratches the surface of de facto
polygamy in western culture. I
suspect that a higher proportion of this polygamous behavior is driven by male
drives and behavior than by female drives and behavior. Men fantasize more about new sexual
partners than women do (by about 18%, according to one large scale
survey).
Another
basically biological difference in male and female mammals is the approach to
sex. Of course, those equipped with a
penis are more likely to be thrusting during copulation, especially in species which copulate
"doggy style". To me, it just seems technically easier for most
mammals, though not at all necessary in
humans of course. And humans like to
have a more imaginative and varied sex life than animals do. Anyway,
all mammals (except humans some of the time) behave in a sex-dimorphic
manner. Females are more passive during
copulation. For example, female rats adopt a species-specific position when
they are ready for copulation: they rest
chest to the ground, raise their rump, and lift their tail to the side. This is called lordosis. So it seems to me that there is something
biological about this trait, and I do
assume that this sex difference does indeed loosely characterize most human
copulation. I vaguely recall that the
Hite report corroborates this in the sense that women are slightly more passive
during copulation -not in the sense that
they adopt lordosis! Indeed, humans seem
to prefer the so-called “missionary” or ventro-ventral position. It has been argued that the missionary
position is sought for some degree of eye contact, a trait we humans are said to share with
dwarf chimpanzees (bonobos) who also copulate in the missionary position (more
so though when they are young, the more
experienced ones preferring the “doggy” style or ventro-dorsal position).
Finally, men and women have a different approach to
the preliminaries of sex. Men are
excited more easily by visual erotica and women seem to prefer petting for a
longer period of time prior to copulation.
Is the determination of these two sex differences biological ? Though there must surely be some cultural
factors involved, I suspect that a
biological sex difference must be at play here as well. Some ethologists (specialists of animal
behavior in natural settings) have proposed that the ecology of the sex drive
is sex-specific: it is in the biological
interest of female mammals to be more selective of mates because they get limited opportunities to
reproduce (gestation takes time).
However, it is in the biological
interest of male mammals to inseminate as many females as possible because the
male genes thus have more chances of passing on to posterity. Of course,
this general account does not apply to all animal species because
ecological pressures are multidetermined and heterogeneous: some species, even among primates (e.g.,
gorillas), form a strong pair bond for life.
Men and women
have sexual relations mostly with the opposite sex. So you would think that nature could have
fine-tuned their sexualities for maximum compatibility. But things are not so simple. Men and women are different sexual
animals. Women have a lower sex
drive, a more passive approach to
copulation, a more stable sexual orientation and sexual identity, many fewer paraphylias (sexual perversions)
and they manifest subtle cycling of sexual desire as a function of the
menstrual cycle (apparently mostly linked primarily to their testosterone
level), and a not so subtle drop in
sexual desire at menopause (see chapter 10).
All of these sex differences could be construed (only by a warped mind,
I think) to be exclusively culturally determined. However, here is an example of a gender
difference in sexual behavior that cannot remotely be construed as cultural: in 1994 a team of American researchers
sprayed exaltolide (an industrial version of androstenol, a human male steroid
hormone) on a chair in a waiting
room. The women nearly all chose to sit
on that chair and the men nearly all avoided sitting on that chair ! None were aware of the odor. The physiology of this mechanism, and its sexual potency, are well known in the
hamster and they are just starting to be investigated in the human. I suspect that we will be hearing a lot more
about this phenomenon as the molecules in question approach commercialization. Let me dwell a little on the subject of one
particular male-prevalent sexual deviation,
namely rape. Recidivist rapists
are sometimes offered a hormonal “solution” to their “problem”, namely antiandrogen treatment or
castration. One study reviewed and
followed up a large set of such cases. Castration causes androgen deficiency
and a decline in sexual behavior. Results showed greatly reduced recidivism
rates for sex offenders who have been castrated. Antiandrogen and hormonal
treatments are pharmacological methods of reducing the sex drive in sexual
offenders. Cyproterone acetate (CPA) is a commercially available antiandrogen
that has been used in the treatment of paraphilias. Many patients have reported
a feeling of calm as a result of CPA treatment as well as reduced sex drive and
reduced deviant fantasies and behaviors.
Human males castrated before puberty fail to develop sexual desire
entirely. Women do not lose their
libido much after ovariectomy, they lose it more after adrenalectomy. This is because the more critical androgens
for female libido are secreted by her adrenal gland. It is still not clear whether estrogen
contributes anything at all to female libido.
The most
spectacular effects of hormones on sexual behavior have frequently been
observed in rats. Prenatal or perinatal
or even postnatal injections of sex hormones can produce gender reversal in
sexual behavior of rats. This is not
observed nearly as much in primates such as monkeys and humans. Rats seem to have a special brain circuitry
for sex. Male and female rats naturally manifest both the
male-specific (mounting) and female-specific (lordosis) sexual behaviors, in different proportions. It is as if they have more androgynous
sexual brains. These circuits can be
easily masculinized or feminized with sex hormone injections, and other manipulations such as
castration, pharmacological
neurotransmitter manipulation, etc. I wish to caution the reader in
overextending inferences about the biology of sexual behavior from rat
research. Monkey research can generally
be considered more trustworthy for extrapolation (which must nevertheless
remain very reserved) to humans. That
is why it is important to note that prenatal androgenization of female macaques
does indeed result in more mounting and leg clasping behavior in young
adulthood, but it is only the proportion
of male-typical and female-typical behaviors which changes.
Post Comment
No comments