Are men superior in visuospatial skills ?
It is commonly stated
that men have superior visuospatial skills (maccoby & Jacklin, 1974). This is also an overstatement. While it is true that men are indeed
significantly better on certain mental rotation tasks, and discrimination of angles, there are
several tasks requiring visuospatial analysis where it is women who are
slightly superior. For example, women slightly outperform men on digit-symbol
coding tasks, tasks which typically form
one of the so-called "Performance" IQ subtests. Such tasks require matching of
pseudo-geometric symbols (their discrimination, recognition and copy require
visuospatial processing) with numbers by looking at exemplar pairs and
completing matches of numbers by drawing in the missing symbols as fast as possible. I have a feeling (it is a gratuitous
speculation) that if the time constraint
were removed, men would achieve a small
advantage. However, even when there is no time constraint, women do as well as men on some visuospatial
tasks. Rey's Complex Figure Test
provides a case in point. This test
comprises a very complex figure comprising many angles and numerous different
frames. As far as my intuition goes, this is a very "visuospatial"
test. The figure must first be
copied, and after several minutes of
delay, it must be drawn from memory. There is no time constraint with regard to
the time available for drawing. Women do
as well as men on this test. I suppose social gender-role specialists would
love to argue that men could very well have superior mechanical reasoning
abilities, and by extension,
visuospatial abilities, because of their
childhood and adult life experiences (taking apart toys for example). But here's a challenge for the social
gender-role approach: If shown a beacon
with a liquid in it, a ground line, and a water level, men will detect more often correctly and
more rapidly whether the water line is correctly parallel to the ground-line or
not. Who would seriously want to argue
that men have more experience pouring liquids from beacons or containers ? Beer drinking does not qualify when one
drinks from the bottle... Men are
frequently found to have a just barely significant advantage on visual
disembedding tasks. Such tasks require
that geometric forms (such as a three dimensional cube) be discerned even
though they are hidden or embedded in a complex arrangement of lines and
angles. In my own research, I have been compelled to recognize the male
superiority in at least one aspect of visuospatial processing. In an investigation of 175 men and 175
women, very well matched for age and
education, I found that men were
significantly superior on a test called the Porteus Mazes test. In this test the subject must draw a line
from the center of a complex maze, avoiding
numerous dead end channels, to finally emerge at one extremity of the
maze. The test can be completed most efficiently if the person
is able to visualize the whole path,
keep it in memory, and then draw
the trajectory in a single go by duplicating the mental image. Though these were very large groups, the sex difference was barely
significant. Yet to my surprise, it transcended another much touted sex
difference, namely the female advantage in verbal fluency. Indeed these same subjects had completed a
verbal fluency test called the Controlled Oral Word Association Test
(COWAT). Though the women did
better, the sex difference did not reach
the threshold for statistical significance. Years later, I came to realize that Porteus himself had
observed the same sex difference on his own test when he compiled his own
norms. This sex difference has often
been ignored in the mainstream literature.
The advantage of men on mental rotation tasks is probably the best
established male advantage. Tests which
show this sex difference are of several sorts.
Several tests of visual aspects of mechanical reasoning have been found
to yield this effect. Another procedure
consists of depicting a number of tridimensional (height, length, depth) block
constructions rotated along one or two axes (right/left, front/back) and requiring the subject to
identify which assemblies are the same and which are different. Finally,
one test presents to the subject pictures of tridimensional cubes with
one or several black or white sides out of the total of six. The subject must then look at unidimensional
models, resembling flat paper cut-outs of the cubes, as if prior to their being folded into the
tridimensional cubes. The subject is
required to discriminate identities and differences between the uni and
tridimensional versions.
A biological determination of male superiority for
visuospatial processing, would seem far
more plausible to me if it were consistently observed in other species. Don't you feel the same way ? Male rats consistently learn to negotiate a
maze (labyrinth) more quickly and with fewer errors than females, -even though there are many other forms of
learning where they have no superiority to females (object discrimination, etc.).
It is even more interesting to note that a single feminizing injection
of the male rat fetus will prevent him from showing the male-typical advantage
at adulthood. Likewise, a single masculinizing hormonal injection of
the female rat fetus will procure for her the male-typical advantage in maze
learning. However, not all of the hormonal determination of this
sex difference is prenatal (organizational).
One study found that the male rat advantage in spatial learning could be
reversed by a single dose of estrogen applied to adults. Furthermore,
one recent study found that female rats are worse at estrous than at
diestrous, and that female rats show a decrement
in spatial learning following ovariectomy (removal of the ovaries). Hormonal determination of cognitive differences
between the sexes is an ongoing affair throughout life. Researchers have even been able to show that
female rats, like women, prefer to navigate in a maze by using
object-markers situated along the trajectory,
-whereas male rats, like men, prefer
to rely on coordinate-space markers, and
ignore the objects along the way.
Here's what I mean by use of "coordinate-space" markers. You want to go visit aunt Harriet at the
other end of town. You have been there
once before, ten years ago. You don't bother looking at the map, or
asking for directions. You hop into your
car, and rely on your image of the
trajectory -which in your memory is a mere vector (direction). You figure she lives exactly Northwest from
you ten miles away, so you just head out
in that general direction, doing the
right and left turns often enough and regularly enough to keep you on that
vectorial track. I tried to find
evidence in the literature of a spatial advantage of male monkeys. It seems that such important research, on the specific theme of visuospatial
ability, remains wanting. The rat model
of cognitive sex differences in visuospatial ability is extremely useful and
convenient but it would be truly useful and important to carry out such studies
with monkeys -a wonderful doctoral
thesis for the taking. Even the rat
model is problematic. For example, because there exists an intimate link between
estrogen and androgen metabolism in the brain, masculinizing effects of
estrogen injections on brain anatomy [SDN-POA] and behavior have been found by
the neuroendocrinologist Dohler. And two
researchers named Williams and Meck found that even lordosis and maze learning
could be maculinized in female rats with injections of estrogen. So in fact,
our knowledge of the effects of prenatal manipulation of the brain with
hormones remains piecemeal. The effects
probably depend on the brain site, the
developmental timing, the dose, the
molecular variant of the injections, and
of course on specific behaviors investigated.
As I explain elsewhere in this book,
virtually nothing is known about such phenomena in humans, aside from a few studies on humans exposed
prenatally to the synthetic estrogen diethylstilbestrol.
If spatial ability is determined by prenatal hormonal
influences on the development of the brain in humans, as it most likely is in
rats, then human females hormonally
masculinized before birth (either within the normal range or beyond) ought to
show a visuospatial superiority compared to their unaffected sisters. Conversely,
men with partial prenatal hormonal feminization should be weaker on
visuospatial tasks. This is roughly what
has been reported. Eleanor Maccoby and
her colleagues measured androgen levels in the umbilical cords of normal
mothers and then tested the cognitive abilities of the progeny 6 years after
birth. The only relation found was for
high androgen levels (in the female range) to correlate positively with (i.e.,
to predict) high visuospatial ability in girls. In a more recent investigation
by Gina Grimshaw and her colleagues, relations were examined between prenatal
testosterone levels in 2nd trimester amniotic fluid and lateralization of
speech, affect, and handedness at age 10.
Girls with higher prenatal testosterone levels were more strongly
right-handed and had stronger left-hemisphere speech representation. Boys with
higher prenatal testosterone levels had stronger right-hemisphere
specialization for the recognition of emotion. The authors claimed that this
pattern of results is most consistent with Witelson's (1991) claim that
prenatal testosterone leads to greater lateralization of function. One study has investigated boys with very low
androgens at puberty (presumably of prenatal origin). These boys had significantly weak
visuospatial abilities. Women whose
mothers had adrenal hyperplasia,
resulting in excessive secretion of androgens into the blood stream and
thus moderately and abnormally masculinizing the fetus, have been found to manifest superior visuospatial
abilities -characteristic of men. Boys born to diabetic mothers treated
during pregnancy with high levels of estrogen had lower visuospatial ability
than controls. Studies of testicular-feminized humans (androgen insensitivity
syndrome, see chapter 7) and of
kwashiorkor protein deficiency testicular feminization syndrome have both found
that so-affected men have a feminine cognitive profile: relatively higher verbal than spatial
abilities. Kwashiorkor is due to a
certain type of undernourishment (protein deficiency). The liver of so affected boys cannot break
down estrogen as efficiently as normal boys.
Estrogen then accumulates and partly feminizes the body, including the brain.
Post Comment
No comments