The neuropsychology of transsexualism
I have stated at the
beginning of this book that it is the prenatal hormone perfusion which is the
more critical in establishing sexual identity and sexual orientation, and probably most of the subtle psychological
differences between the sexes as well.
One way to help figure out the extent to which sex differences can
result from a secondary hormone bath, at adolescence or even later, is the study of psychological, behavioral and
neuropsychological differences between people before and after a sex change
-which always includes hormone treatment as an adjunct. It turns out, not so surprisingly, that one of the traits which depends on the
hormones of adulthood is sexual desire.
Men who become phenotypic women report a decrease in libido and
aggressiveness and women who become phenotypic men report an increase in libido
and aggressiveness. But more
intriguingly, there are more subtle
traits which switch around as well,
traits which these people could not be aware of -even if they wanted
to. Some studies have found that the
sex-change operation significantly redistributes the verbal and visuospatial
aptitudes in the direction of the targeted sex, -even though the respondents
are not aware that such sex differences exist (Van-Goozen et al, 1995). One investigation comprised
an evaluation of 22 individuals (mean age 25 years) seeking sex-change surgery
and diagnosed as transsexual by a Gender Identity Research Team. These persons underwent extensive
neuropsychological and cognitive testing prior to surgery. On the Wechsler
Adult Intelligence Scale, the subjects performed in congruence with their
biologic sex rather than gender identity. In contrast, on a measure of
conceptual styles, the subjects performed in congruence with their gender
identity. Recall that homosexual men
tend to be last borns. And recall that all maternal steroïd hormones are in
lesser supply with each pregnancy. Maternal supply of testosterone during
gestation must perhaps be high enough (among other things) for normal sexual
orientation to be implanted in her male fetus’s brain. What then is the situation with
transsexuals ? Homosexual male gender dysphorics (male transsexuals who prefer
men as sexual partners) have a later
birth order: each older brother increases odds by 45% (similar to a prior study
for gay men). Male heterosexual transsexuals have an opposite effect: each
older brother decreases the odds! Regarding birth order, then, homosexual male
gender dysphorics are more like homosexual men without gender dysphoria than
they are to heterosexual male gender dysphorics. The situation is much less
complicated for biological females with gender dysphoria, who are almost always
attracted to biological females, though
the very small minority who are attracted to biological males have been much
less well studied. All of this supports
the notion, explained above, according to which the maternal hormonal
status (particularly testosterone) during pregnancy influences development of
brain circuits responsible for sexual orientation in male offspring.
To me, such findings are very important in
constraining our neurobiological theories of sex differences. My reading of the literature on the
neurobiology of sex differences is that it is too dry, not wet enough. Let me explain. Wet physiology is the physiology of fluids
and chemicals in the body -also known as
pharmacology. There are complex interactions
between steroid hormones and neurotransmitters in the brain. Some of these interactions are fixed before
birth. Some of these interactions have a
structuring effect on brain development.
However, some of these
interactions continue to take place and to influence the mind and behavior
throughout life. I will try to make a
case for this point of view in the upcoming section on the neuropsychology of
the menstrual cycle. Incidentally,
you may be wondering what “dry”
physiology might be ? It is electrophysiology.
Most studies of transsexuals do not compare the person
to himself (or herself) before and after the sex-change operation. It is important to understand that when the
research design is not a pre-post operation design, what is being studied is more likely to
result from particularities of transsexuals' brains which were in fact
prenatally determined and fixed. One
large scale study of transsexuals (Orlebeke et al, 1992) investigated
handedness -but not before and after any
sex-change operation. A statistically significant incidence of left handedness
was found. In fact, the transsexuals were twice more at risk for
sinistrality (left handedness) than were the heterosexual controls. Interestingly, the largest handedness study in homosexuals
that I am aware of, carried out on 1,612
cases, found an incidence of left
handedness which was just barely statistically significant (Becker et al, 1992). The authors of the study
happened (to their credit) to mention that a proportion of their sample was
also transsexual in addition to being homosexual, and of course, as is usually the case with these studies,
all the subjects were men. The higher
incidence of sinistrality in transsexuals is a finding which has been recently
replicated by several research teams.
It can therefore be considered quite robust. Incidentally,
the latest of this string of studies has found that female-to-male
transsexuals (not just the more commonly studied male-to-female transsexuals)
are also significantly more often sinistral than either sex with the standard
sexual identity. Another interesting
recent finding is that male-to-female transsexuals have finger ridge asymetry
(of the right and left hand) which significantly differs form men with a normal
sexual identity. However, female-to-male transsexuals do not differ
from women with a normal sexual identity.
One study looked at dichotic listening in male-to-female operated
transsexuals (Cohen
& Forget, 1995).
It found that the profile of ear asymmetries was female-typical, that
is, less asymmetric than observed in normal men. Such findings could be explained just as
easily in terms of interhemispheric communication as in terms of hemispheric
specialization. However, the one brain imaging study that I am aware
of which looked at the corpus callosum (the major commissure linking the two
hemispheres of the brain) in transsexuals and controls found no difference at
all (Emory
et al, 1991).
On the other hand, the sampling
was a bit crude in that all subjects were right handed, and sex differences in corpus callosum
morphology are typically observed only when handedness variation is also taken
into account.
Post Comment
No comments