The Central Nervous System
Physiologically, glucose
is the major nutrient for the brain. Therefore, the undisturbed supply and
metabolism of glucose in the brain, is of central significance. A role for
insulin in regulation of this process by acting in the brain is becoming
increasingly acknowledged. In addition to insulin crossing the blood-brain
barrier (BBB) by a saturation dependent transport mechanism, a small proportion
of brain insulin is produced in the brain itself by pyramidal neurons residing
in the hippocampus, prefrontal cortex, entorhinal cortex and the olfactory bulb
(Hrytsenko et al., 2007; Madadi et al., 2008; Plata-Salaman,
1991; Singh et al., 1997). Insulin gene expression and insulin synthesis have
been demonstrated in both immature and mature mammalian neuronal cells (Schechter and Abboud, 2001; Schechter et al., 1992).
Insulin mRNA was found to be distributed in the brain, with the highest density
in pyramidal cells of the hippocampus and high densities in the prefrontal
cortex, the enthorhinal and perihinal bulb, as well as in the hypothalamus. Two
different types of IRs have been found in the adult mammalian brain, a
neuron-specific brain type with an α-subunit of 118 kDa and a β-subunit of 91 kDa which is
not downregulated by insulin, and a peripheral type on glial cells with an α-subunit
of 130 kDa and a β-subunit of 95kDa which is downregulated by insulin (Adamo et al., 1989). In the brain, insulin’s various functions are
dependent on IR distribution. For example, in the hypothalamic regions,
signaling through the IR appears to be vitally involved in the regulation of
food intake and body weight (Guthoff et al., 2010; Plum et al., 2006).
However, in the higher limbic system, including the hippocampus, signaling through
the IR has been involved in mediating biochemical stimuli necessary for
synaptic activity, plasticity and memory function (Guzowski et al., 2000; Kremerskothen et al., 2002;
Lannert and Hoyer, 1998;
Park, 2001;
Steward et al., 1998;
Zhao et al., 1999).
Several studies show that hippocampal cognitive
performance may be dependent on an adequate supply of glucose to meet the
metabolic demands of cognitive processes, and that a sufficient supply of
glucose to the hippocampus can enhance memory performance (Gold, 2005; McNay et al., 2000; McNay and Gold, 2002). Similar to its effects in the periphery, insulin has been
thought to stimulate translocation of GLUTs to the membrane of hippocampal
neurons, indicating that insulin may increase hippocampal glucose uptake and/or
metabolism (McEwen and Reagan, 2004; Reagan, 2005; Thorens and Mueckler, 2010). Several in vitro studies are consistent with a
role for insulin in modulation of synaptic plasticity (Izumi et al., 2003; van der Heide et al., 2005;
Zhao et al., 2004). Hence, there are substantial reasons for
believing that hippocampal functions such as learning and memory may in part be
dependent on insulin acting in the brain. In support of this hypothesis, recent
animal studies have suggested that delivery of insulin to the hippocampus may
be able to modulate hippocampal memory processes. However, these studies have
mostly used insulin doses that exceed the physiological situation or aversive
methods with elevated systemic glucose and epinephrine, making interpretation
of a specific insulin effect
difficult (Babri et al., 2007; Moosavi et al., 2007; Moosavi et al., 2006). Nevertheless, a recent study by McNay and colleagues shows that
delivery of physiological
doses of insulin, but not insulin like growth factor (IGF-1), directly to the
hippocampus, specifically
enhances spatial working memory via a PI3K-dependent mechanism (McNay et al., 2010). Administration of insulin also increased local
glucose removal from interstitial fluid, whereas blockade of endogenous
hippocampal insulin action, either
with PI3K antagonists or small anti-insulin antibody-like peptides, impaired cognitive
performance below baseline. These data are in support of a role for insulin in
physiological hippocampal memory
processes.
As the brain consumes 18-30% of total body
glucose (Magistretti, 2006), disruptions in glucose supply and utilization
regulated by insulin signaling, can easily result in neuronal dysfunction or
damage. Indeed, in the developing brain, hyperglycemia and hyperinsulemia are
accompanied by neuronal death, whereas severe chronic hypoglycemia can lead to
permanent brain damage and mental retardation (Dunne et al., 2004). Insulin resistance and the sometimes resulting
T2DM have long been associated with impaired cognitive function (Awad et al., 2004; Biessels et al., 2008;
McNay et al., 2010;
Stewart and Liolitsa, 1999;
Strachan et al., 1997), thus it is
crucial to know what happens to insulin signaling in the brain, as a
consequence of insulin resistance and peripheral hyperinsulemia. Insulin is
readily transported into the CNS across the BBB by a saturable,
receptor-mediated process (Simpson et al., 2007). Based on a study regarding the kinetics of CNS insulin
uptake after dexamethasone treatment (Baura et al., 1996), Craft postulates that peripheral
insulin resistance and subsequent hyperinsulinemia does not promote parallel
increases of insulin in the brain and actually reduces transport into the brain
(Craft, 2006). Although
direct evidence for this assumption is still lacking, this could indicate that in
insulin resistant subjects, insulin signaling in the brain may be deficient due
to reduced levels of insulin. Contradictory, Craft
and colleagues have also reported that intravenous insulin administration in
humans induces transport of insulin into the CNS (Craft, 2006; Craft et al., 2003),
which would be more conceivable, as insulin is transported through the BBB by a
saturable mechanism.
Due to
loss of lean body mass and an increase in adipose tissue during ageing, less
muscle tissue and relatively more adipose tissue is available for glucose clearance,
thereby increasing the risk for insulin resistance in genetic susceptible
individuals. Consistent
with reduced brain insulin levels as a result of increased insulin resistance and
hyperinsulemia with due to ageing, Frölich and colleagues reported that brain insulin
levels are decreased in elderly, as is the number of IRs (Frolich et al., 1998). Interestingly, they also
showed that in SAD patients, compared to age-matched controls, the number of IRs
was increased in all regions of the brain cortex, with a significant increase
in the occipital cortex. The authors suggest that this finding is due to an
upregulation of the IR on top of a general loss of IRs with ageing, potentially
because of impaired downstream signal transduction. Accordingly, Frölich and
colleagues also reported that IR tyrosine kinase activity was reduced in SAD
patients compared to both middle-aged and age-matched controls. These data are
in support of the hypothesis that impaired insulin signaling may represent an
important pathogenic event in AD.
In contrast to studies reporting that brain insulin
levels decrease with age, as well as in AD, some have reported that insulin
levels rise with ageing and are strong predictors of cognitive decline in
adults without diabetes (Stolk et al., 1997a; Stolk et al., 1997b). As the authors note, these
findings appeared to be compatible with a direct effect of insulin on the brain.
Also, these results are to be expected as insulin resistance is associated with
ageing, and is likely to lead to increased brain insulin levels. An in vivo study
by Gerozissis and colleagues, further demonstrated that brain insulin levels
are regulated locally and are not always dependent of pancreatic insulin
regulation, further emphasizing the complexity of the regulatory process of
brain insulin levels and downstream signal transduction (Gerozissis et al., 2001). Correspondingly, different studies characterized and
quantified AD-associated abnormalities in insulin, IGF and IR gene expression,
and associated downstream signaling mechanisms using human AD and age-matched
control post mortem brain samples (Rivera et al., 2005; Steen et al., 2005).
They found that insulin-, IGF-I-, IR-, and IGF-I receptor-positive neurons were
less abundant in the AD brain compared with the age-matched control samples.
Reduced labeling of neurons in AD cases appeared attributable to both loss of
neurons as well as reduced neuronal expression. Furthermore, the authors
reported that levels of activated AKT (phospho-AKT) were also reduced in the AD
brain, in contrast to GSK-3β activity which was increased. Combined, these studies
convincingly implicate aberrant insulin signaling in the pathophysiology of AD.
In
summary, to elucidate to which extent insulin signaling is misregulated during
ageing, or contributes to AD pathology, it is important to understand how
insulin signaling is affected as a result of ageing, or as a possible cause or downstream
effect in the pathogenesis of AD (see fig. 2). Most data appear to be in
agreement with the notion that downstream signaling through the IR decreases
with ageing and is further impaired in AD. Therefore it is possible that the
increased risk for AD as a result of T2DM or insulin resistance, is related to
reduced output of insulin signaling pathways in AD associated brain areas. Interestingly,
streptozotocin-induced insulin deficiency promoted cerebral amyloidosis and
cognitive deficits in an Alzheimer transgenic mouse model expressing a chimeric
mouse/human APP (MO/HuAPP695swe) and a mutant human presenilin1 (PSEN1dE9) gene
(Wang et al., 2010b). Nevertheless, knockout of the neuronal IR appeared
not sufficient to cause cognitive deficits or neuronal loss in mice, even
though tau hyperphosphorylation was observed, indicating that impaired insulin
signaling must perhaps interact with other pathogenic mechanisms for the
development of AD. Moreover, evidence suggests that downstream insulin
signaling is not simply reduced, but that insulin signaling components may be
aberrantly translocated (Cole and Frautschy, 2007). Consistent with an aberrant activation and
translocation event, phospho-ser473AKT was found to be increased in membrane
particulate fractions, but reduced in cytosolic fractions from AD brains (Griffin et al., 2005).
In conclusion, it remains difficult to reliably evaluate phosphorylation
patterns in postmortem AD and control brain, and more importantly, it remains
to be demonstrated that aberrant fluctuations in brain insulin levels and/or
signaling in AD, are high enough to be functionally significant to neurons.
Post Comment
No comments