Genomic imprinting: A revolution in conceptualisation of sex differences.
Sexual imprinting of genes or portions of chromosomes is part of a
more general phenomenon called "genomic imprinting." Broadly speaking, it refers to reversible,
differential expression of a gene or set of genes in an offspring individual
according to whether the genes came from the mother or from the father. It's a very widespread and normal process,
with a large literature. Apparently the phenomenon was first discovered in
insects. Genetic imprinting is defined as a reversible, differential marking of
genes or chromosomes that is determined by the sex of the parent from whom the
genetic material is inherited. Imprinting was first observed in insects where,
in some species, most notably among the coccoids (scale insects and allies),
the differential marking of paternally and maternally transmitted chromosome
sets leads to inactivation or elimination of paternal chromosomes. Imprinting
is also widespread in plants and mammals, in which paternally and maternally
inherited alleles may be differentially expressed. Despite imprinting having
been discovered in insects, clear examples of parental imprinting are scarce in
the model insect species Drosophila melanogaster. There is however a case of
imprint-mediated control of gene expression in Drosophila. The imprinted gene -
the white+ eye-color gene - is expressed at a low level when transmitted by
males, and at a high level when transmitted by females. Thus, in common with
coccoids, Drosophila is capable of generating an imprint, and can respond to
that imprint by silencing the paternal allele. Golic (1998) has recently
published a fascinating review of this litterature.
One of the basic mechanisms of mammalian genomic
imprinting seems to be differences in the methylation of cytosine residues in
the DNA in developing sperm and egg.
Note that this occurs in the parents.
The result is that although the sperm and egg carry the same genes --
that is, the same base sequences in their DNA -- these sequences have been
handled differently during the development of the egg and of the sperm. The result is stated blandly by Scott
Gilbert: "It appears that during germ cell formation, previous information
is erased, and then, during meiosis, new information is introduced into the
genome. The pattern of methylation on a
given gene can differ between egg and sperm, and these gene-specific methylation
differences are seen in the chromosomes of the embryonic cells. Thus, methylation differences between sperm
and egg genes may specify whether a gene came from the father or mother. This
maternal and paternal imprinting adds additional information to the inherited
genomes, information that may regulate spatial and temporal gene activity and
chromosome behavior. Gilbert concludes even more blandly that gamete-specific
methylation and the resulting genomic imprinting "... also provides a reminder that the
organism cannot be explained solely by its genes. One needs knowledge of developmental
parameters as well as genetic ones."
The Prader-Willi and Angelman syndromes are textbook
examples of genomically imprinted disorders in humans. In humans, the loss of a particular gene in
the long arm of chromosome 15 results in different phenotypes depending on
whether the loss is in the male-derived or the female-derived chromosome. If the defective or missing gene comes from
the father, the child is born with Prader-Willi syndrome, a disease associated
with mild mental retardation, obesity, small gonads, and short stature. If the defective or missing gene comes from
the mother, the child has Angelman syndrome, characterized by severe mental
retardation, lack of speech, and inappropriate laughter (see Gilbert, 1994).
Prader-Willi syndrome (PWS) and Angelman syndrome (AS) are distinct
neurodevelopmental disorders with interrelated genetic mechanisms because
genomic imprinting within the chromosome 15q11-13 region affects both the PWS
and the AS locus. One study carried out by Buchholz and colleagues (1998)
presents clinical and molecular data on a large series of 258 referred
patients, evaluated with methylation analysis: 115 with suspected PWS and 143
with suspected AS. In these patients, the clinical phenotype was graded into
three groups: classical (group 1); not classical but possible (group 2); not
classical and unlikely (group 3). For
PWS, a fourth group consisted of hypotonic babies. DNA methylation analysis confirmed the
diagnosis of PWS in 30 patients (26%) and AS in 28 patients (20%). For 21 PWS
patients the mechanism was established: 15 had deletions, 4 had uniparental
disomy (UPD) and 2 a presumed imprinting defect. Clinically all those with an
abnormal methylation pattern had the classical phenotype and none of those with
a normal methylation pattern had classical PWS. For 23 AS patients in whom a
mechanism was established, 17 had a deletion, 3 had UPD and 3 had a presumed
imprinting defect. There was greater clinical overlap in AS, with 26 classical
AS patients having a normal methylation pattern while an abnormal methylation
pattern was seen in one patient from group 2. In addition, there were a further
40 patients with a normal methylation pattern in whom AS was still a possible
diagnosis. Our conclusion is that methylation analysis provides an excellent
screening test for both syndromes, providing approximately 99% diagnosis for
PWS and for AS, a 75% diagnostic rate, supplemented for the remaining 25% with
an essential basic starting point to further investigations.
Though Prader-Willi and Angelman syndromes were among
the first brain disorders to be identified as involving genomic
imprinting, human genetic studies have
since directed attention to genomic imprinting in a number of syndromes
involving brain dysfunction, such as congenital adrenal hyperplasia, Turner's
syndrome, bipolar depression, dyslexia,
autism, schizophrenia and even homosexuality. Molecular genetics is
providing insight into the complexity of these imprinting mechanisms, while
experimental studies are revealing the differential roles that maternal and
paternal genomes may play in brain development and growth.
Congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH) is an inherited
recessive disorder of adrenal steroidogenesis caused by mutations in the
steroid 21-hydroxylase gene (CYP21) in more than 90% of affected patients. The
CYP21 gene is located within the HLA complex locus on chromosome 6 (6p21.3).
During a molecular characterisation study of a group of 47 Mexican families with
21-hydroxylase deficiency, Lopez-Gutierrez and colleagues (1998) identified
nine in which the mutation or mutations found in the patient did not appear to
originate from one of the parents. Through DNA fingerprinting, paternity was
established in all nine families with a probability of non-paternity in the
range of 10(-19) to 10(-23). Among these families, Lopez-Gutierrez and
colleagues identified one patient with exclusive paternal inheritance of all
eight markers tested on chromosome 6p, despite normal maternal and paternal
contributions for eight additional markers on three different chromosomes.
Lopez-Gutierrez and colleagues did not identify duplication of paternal
information for markers in the 6q region, consistent with lack of expression of
transient neonatal diabetes owing to genomic imprinting in this patient. These
results substantiate evidence for the existence of different genetic mechanisms
involved in the expression of this recessive condition in a substantial portion
(approximately 19%) of affected Mexican families. In addition to the
identification of a patient with paternal uniparental disomy, the occurrence of
germline mutations may explain the unusual pattern of segregation in the
majority of the remaining eight families.
Turner's syndrome is a sporadic disorder of human
females in which all or part of one X chromosome is deleted. Intelligence is
usually normal but social adjustment problems are common. Skuse and colleagues
(1997) recently published a study of 80
females with Turner's syndrome and a single X chromosome. In 55 cases the X was maternally derived
(45,X[m]) and in 25 it was of paternal origin (45,X[p]). Members of the 45,X[p]
group were significantly better adjusted, with superior verbal and higher-order
executive function skills, which mediate social interactions. These
observations suggest that there is a genetic locus for social cognition, which
is imprinted and is not expressed from the maternally derived X chromosome.
Neuropsychological and molecular investigations of eight females with partial
deletions of the short arm of the X chromosome indicate that the putative
imprinted locus escapes X-inactivation, and probably lies on Xq or close to the
centromere on Xp. If expressed only from the X chromosome of paternal origin, the
existence of this locus could explain why 46,XY males (whose single X
chromosome is maternal) are more vulnerable to developmental disorders of
language and social cognition, such as autism, than are 46,XX females.
Skuse and colleagues have even proposed that basic
normal sex differences in cognitive ability,
women’s verbal advantage and men’s spatial advantage for example, are also tributary to genomic imprinting. This puts into question classical doctrine
according to which the sole vector of gender-specific brain
differentiation, aside from
experience, is hormonal. This represents a revolutionary way of
thinking about biological bases of brain sex differences.
Post Comment
No comments